榴莲视频

Questionable techniques ‘rife’ in ecological research

<榴莲视频 class="standfirst">Study suggests reproducibility crisis may not be limited to psychology and biomedicine
七月 31, 2018
A crossroads
Source: Getty
At a crossroads: questionable ecological research unleashed a trail of waste as research funds were frittered on ‘dead ends’

The dubious practices that plague psychological research are equally prevalent in some natural sciences, suggesting that the “reproducibility crisis” may not be limited to psychology and biomedicine.

An Australian-led study has found that dodgy tricks of the trade –?such as cherry-picking data, ignoring unwanted results and touting unexpected findings as though they had been predicted all along – are widespread in ecological and evolutionary research.

The findings reinforce fears that a “publish or perish” culture and a publication bias towards positive results are undermining research integrity and planting false leads in the scientific literature.

Lead author Hannah Fraser said that activities formerly considered research transgressions were now becoming standard practice. While the consequences?are not as “problematic” as they are in biomedicine – where billions of dollars?have been squandered on fruitless clinical trials – she said that questionable ecological research unleashed a trail of waste as research funds were frittered on “dead ends”.

“People spend a lot of time chasing their tails,” said Dr Fraser, a University of Melbourne ecologist. The problem tainted science’s overall credibility, giving policymakers a “neat excuse” to ignore inconvenient findings.

The study, in the journal Plos One, was based on a confidential survey of more than 800 authors published in 21 ecology and evolution journals. It investigated activities that lie outside acceptable scientific procedure but mostly fall short of outright fraud.

Respondents assessed the prevalence of such behaviour among peers and confessed how frequently they themselves had transgressed. While similar studies have been conducted into psychology research, this is thought to be the first to focus on ecology and evolution.

Almost two in three respondents confessed to having nudged their findings into statistical significance by reporting some and ignoring others. More than two in five said that they had collected additional data after finding that the initially planned observations had failed to deliver statistically significant results – an activity known as data dredging or “p-丑补肠办颈苍驳”.

Just over half acknowledged having pretended that unexpected findings supported their original premises – a practice dubbed “hypothesising after the results are known”, or “HARKing”.

The frequencies of questionable practices were similar to those uncovered in two recent studies into psychological research, contradicting the squeaky clean self-image of natural sciences. “Ecologists are likely to say, ‘there’s a problem in psychology, but we do things differently,’?” Dr Fraser said.

Some respondents whitewashed their behaviour, with one blaming HARKing on editors’ demands for exploratory results to be “framed as a?priori hypotheses”. Another justified rounding off figures to achieve statistical significance, accusing researchers who reported their data precisely of “adherence to conventional practice over understanding of probability”.

Dr Fraser said that she herself had done “a whole lot of questionable things” when she tweaked findings from a master’s project to make them more “palatable” after reviewers from several journals had criticised her manuscript.

“Looking back, it was essentially a big p-hacking experience,” she said. “It didn’t feel like it because I?was just responding to reviewer comments. I?suspect that kind of thing happens a?lot.”

john.ross@timeshighereducation.com

请先注册再继续

为何要注册?

  • 注册是免费的,而且十分便捷
  • 注册成功后,您每月可免费阅读3篇文章
  • 订阅我们的邮件
注册
Please 登录 or 注册 to read this article.
<榴莲视频 class="pane-title"> 相关大学
ADVERTISEMENT