榴莲视频

Academics’ unscientific assertions on Covid are exacerbating division

<榴莲视频 class="standfirst">The pandemic has demonstrated that there are broad deficiencies in quantitative reasoning skills even within the academy, says David Sanders
一月 5, 2022
A Republican-led forum on the origins of the Covid-19 virus at the US Capitol on 29 June 2021
Source: Getty

As if the direct effects of Covid-19 on public health were not bad enough, the pandemic has also been exploited as an opportunity to advance many socially corrosive agendas. These include xenophobia, science rejection, conspiracy-theory propagation, cultism, antidemocratic agitation, antivaccination propagandising and generalised fearmongering.

Unfortunately, far too many of the academic community are exacerbating the problems. Sometimes their efforts are directed towards countering the antisocial forces; nevertheless, their abandonment of logic and egoless objectivity constitutes a betrayal of the norms of scientific discourse that will ultimately increase public mistrust of science – and loosen humanity’s hold on reality.

One fundamental error is to misunderstand the significance of precedent in science. If a phenomenon has been authenticated in the past then it can recur. However, one cannot generally infer anything about the probability of a natural phenomenon occurring from the fact that it has not previously occurred. This is not to say that there cannot be sound theoretical bases for determining that an event is impossible or unlikely. It simply means that precedent without a solid, determinative scientific concept should not be invoked in argument.

For example, it is claimed that the hypothesis that Sars-CoV-2 (the Covid-19 pathogen) was engineered in a laboratory is supported by its possession of a particular function-enhancing sequence in its spike protein that is unique among the subfamily of coronaviruses to which it belongs. This is logically incorrect. Some coronaviruses in particular subfamilies have such a sequence, and some lack it. Until the first coronavirus in a particular subfamily was found to have such a sequence, none of them were known to do so. Every novel discovery necessarily defies precedent.

Some scientists insist that Sars-CoV-2, whose sequence is closest to that of bat coronaviruses, most likely was transmitted to humans through an intermediate mammalian host because that seems to have been what happened with other novel coronaviruses,?such as severe acute respiratory syndrome (Sars) (although this proposition has been challenged) and Middle East respiratory syndrome (Mers).?But the sample size is too small to draw such an inference, and no scientific principle would be violated if there were direct transmission from bats.

The pandemic has again demonstrated that there are broad deficiencies in quantitative reasoning skills even within the academy. Statistical data have been misinterpreted and misrepresented. Another logic failure is the inference that what appears to be a rare event is unlikely to underlie an observed outcome. For example, it has been averred repeatedly that Sars-CoV-2 is too well adapted to a human host to have arisen naturally. This is utterly absurd and conveys a complete ignorance of virology and epidemiology.

The argument is made that the virus’ greater transmissibility between humans compared with that of Sars or Mers could not have evolved through natural selection. This appeal to intuition is not scientific. The reason our attention is focused on Sars-CoV-2 is precisely that it is readily transmitted among humans and causes severe disease in some patients. Although the combination is rare, it is not improbable that such a virus should arise.

Humans encounter animal virus particles all the time. Most of those do not overcome the defence barriers that protect us – barriers that include one unique to apes and old world monkeys that counters enveloped viruses such as coronaviruses. Even fewer, after infecting a person, have the capacity to be transmitted to others and cause disease. However, given the extent of our interactions with animal populations we are bound to be exposed to some viruses with these properties. And modern urbanisation and travel patterns are conducive to rapid transmission.

What drives scientists to violate professional standards on the judicious weighing of evidence? Two obvious culprits are team identification and confirmation bias. It is true that such cognitive distortions have frequently been present in historical scientific disputes. Nevertheless, the problems are particularly acute in the discussion of the pandemic, which has been characterised by the clash of political and national agendas with public-health advocacy.

It is evident that the loudest voices are those of people who took sides on some issue regarding the outbreak. They broadcast data and opinions from associates that they believe support those initial stands, and they refuse to consider emerging evidence that challenges their position. Scientists who advocate for a balanced approach, maintaining that definite conclusions cannot be drawn from much of the available information, are largely ignored.

Disinformation about Covid-19 is predominantly distributed through social media. Obviously, those who wish to counter the falsehoods cannot abandon the playing field to the conspiracy-mongers. I am one of those who occasionally use social media to try to transmit the facts, and I would certainly prefer if my message were received by as large an audience as possible. Nevertheless, the techniques employed by the most influential voices, including those who are well intentioned, are frequently antithetical to reasoned discourse.

Prominence is often achieved through the provocation of outrage, rather than induction of rationality. The egos of far too many communicators are dependent on their tally of followers and the responses to their postings. In addition, the size of a person’s electronic entourage is mistakenly used even by serious journalists as a proxy for determining whether a person has something worthwhile to say.

The pandemic has exposed numerous fissures in society. The academic community should not be widening them through logical and psychological fallacies, however crowd-pleasing the initial reaction may be.

David A. Sanders is an associate professor of biological sciences at Purdue University in Indiana.

<榴莲视频 class="pane-title"> 后记

Print headline:?Scholars against science

请先注册再继续

为何要注册?

  • 注册是免费的,而且十分便捷
  • 注册成功后,您每月可免费阅读3篇文章
  • 订阅我们的邮件
注册
Please 登录 or 注册 to read this article.
<榴莲视频 class="pane-title"> 相关文章
<榴莲视频 class="pane-title"> Reader's comments (3)
Immensely important commentary. While some of the problems articulated maybe a result of society's love of social media and its algorithms. The communication and accessibility of scientific studies, especially those with the negative outcomes, to a wider audience outside of academia might also have cultivated the soil for the propagation of dangerous social media posts chasing ever more followers.
Great article. Academics have incentives too - publications, impact, visibility - leading to other opportunities, ego , policial and other affiliations among other things. They are not some non partisan, purely rational agent. Although most academics consiously try hard to be as objective as possible but in the cut-throat world of academia (yes you heard that correct) standards slip for some - fudging data, inadequate samples, p-hacking, harcking etc.....
Well said.
ADVERTISEMENT